THE SCIENCE BEHIND ONVY

The Scientific Foundation of our Zones Concept

Recovery influences the Zones of Stress and Activity

Low recovery negatively impacts our ability to cope with stress. Therefore a lower Recovery Score will decrease the recommended amount of Stress (Stress Zone).

Low recovery also negatively influences various aspects of athletic performance. Therefore a lower Recovery Score decreases the recommended amount of activity (Activity Zone).

  • Sleep deprivation impedes regeneration and decreases performance in athletes (Vitale et al. 2019)

 
 

Activity influences the Zones of Recovery and Stress

High activity is always accompanied by a certain amount of damage the body needs to repair. This repair is primarily done during rest (e.g., sleep). Therefore a higher Activity Score increases the amount of recommended recovery  (Recovery Zone).

  • Periodization of activity with adequate rest for optimal, continual improvements (Cunanan et al. 2018)

Physical activity can improve the ability to cope with stress. Therefore a higher Activity Score increases the amount of recommended stress (Stress Zone).

Mindfulness influences the Zone of Stress

Mindfulness can increase the ability to cope with stress. Therefore a higher Mindfulness Score increases the amount of recommended stress (Stress Zone).

 
 

Why is there an optimal Zone for Stress

For most people stress is something that should be reduced, period. We don't agree, because humans actually need and seek a certain amount of stress

Our Research Process concerning Zones and Metrics

Getting reliable and valid data from wearables is a challenge due to the heterogeneity of the market, frequent updates concerning hardware and software, and the inconsistency of measurements because they happen in non-standardized environments (Mühlen et al. 2021). When possible, we prefer data from wearables where a higher validity and reliability have been proven. E.g OURA for sleep measurements (Altini and Kinnunen 2021).

To reduce the probability of errors, we keep as close as possible to the scientific method while being aware that we are in an extremely dynamic environment where we need to provide pragmatic solutions.

  1. We accumulate data through third-party apps and devices like Apple Health or Oura.

  2. We conduct research concerning their scientific validity and reliability. With more consistency concerning study design, in the future, we might be able to use checklists from e.g. the INTERLIVE network (Mühlen et al. 2021)

  3. We formulate our hypothesis

  4. We validate the hypothesis internally and develop and improve our algorithms based on our findings.

  5. The algorithms get integrated into our application and we continuously validate and improve from hereon.

  6. For more vague and subjective concepts like Recovery (“How recovered do I feel?”) and stress (“How stressed do I feel?”), we conduct automatic user assessments to further validate our algorithms.

 Improving Oneself is a Scientific Endeavor

We help people to understand themselves better. To give our users the possibility to run experiments themselves, we based our ONVY process on the scientific method.

 

Option 1: Let the user go through the process himself

1) Observe - Check the ONVY Scores: “My recovery is suboptimal. Also I feel tired during the day.”

2) Research - Dig deeper into the Subscores: “Why is this the case?”

3) Create a Hypothesis - Derive understanding from Subscores: “It seems like I am not sleeping enough. If I sleep longer this might increase my recovery and I will feel less tired during the day.”

4) Test - Do an intervention: “I try to sleep 30 minutes longer”

5) Analyze - Check the ONVY Score: “Does my recovery (objectively and subjectively) improve when I sleep longer?”

6) Report - See if this had led to improvements: “Do I need to do anything else to improve my recovery?”

Option 2: Get AI-based feedback on the scores and receive guidance from ONVY with our Health Programs. In this case, we observe, research, create a hypothesis and recommend an intervention. The intervention is automatically tracked, analyzed, and provided as a comprehensive report to the user.

 
 
 

What is Mental Fitness?

Mental Fitness is the flexible capacity to utilize resources and skills to psychologically adapt to internal and external challenges or advantages to enable optimal functioning (Robinson, Oades, and Caputi 2015). Similar descriptions can be found for the concept of flow (Šimleša et al. 2018). In addition, mental Fitness includes stress management, decision-making, fatigue countermeasures, emotional stability, and emotional awareness (Aidman 2020). Also, physical ability, recovery, and relaxation techniques play an essential role in improving Mental Fitness (Reardon et al., 2019).

How our Metrics are connected to Mental Fitness

Sleep (recovery) deprivation decreases cognitive performance (Csipo et al. 2021), flow experience, and mood (Kaida and Niki 2013).

Higher activity correlates with well-being and flow state experiences (Wiese, Kuykendall, and Tay 2018). Exercise improves cognitive performance (Mandolesi et al. 2018).

The right amount of stress is necessary to achieve peak performance and to feel satisfied (Matthews et al. 2014). 

Higher mindfulness is correlated with life satisfaction (Liang et al. 2022), mental wellbeing (van Agteren et al. 2021), peak performance (Chen et al. 2018), and the propensity to get into a flow state (Moore 2013).

 
 

References

Agteren, Joep van, Matthew Iasiello, Laura Lo, Jonathan Bartholomaeus, Zoe Kopsaftis, Marissa Carey, and Michael Kyrios. 2021. “A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Psychological Interventions to Improve Mental Wellbeing.” Nature Human Behaviour 5 (5): 631–52. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01093-w.

Aidman, Eugene. 2020. “Cognitive Fitness Framework: Towards Assessing, Training and Augmenting Individual-Difference Factors Underpinning High-Performance Cognition.” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00466.

Altini, Marco, and Hannu Kinnunen. 2021. “The Promise of Sleep: A Multi-Sensor Approach for Accurate Sleep Stage Detection Using the Oura Ring.” Sensors (Basel, Switzerland) 21 (13): 4302. https://doi.org/10.3390/s21134302.

Chen, Jian-Hong, Po-Hsin Tsai, Yin-Chou Lin, Chih-Ken Chen, and Ching-Yen Chen. 2018. “Mindfulness Training Enhances Flow State and Mental Health among Baseball Players in Taiwan.” Psychology Research and Behavior Management 12 (December): 15–21. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S188734.

Choi, Dong-Woo, Sung-Youn Chun, Sang Ah Lee, Kyu-Tae Han, and Eun-Cheol Park. 2018. “Association between Sleep Duration and Perceived Stress: Salaried Worker in Circumstances of High Workload.” International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 15 (4): 796. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15040796.

Csipo, Tamas, Agnes Lipecz, Cameron Owens, Peter Mukli, Jonathan W. Perry, Stefano Tarantini, Priya Balasubramanian, et al. 2021. “Sleep Deprivation Impairs Cognitive Performance, Alters Task-Associated Cerebral Blood Flow and Decreases Cortical Neurovascular Coupling-Related Hemodynamic Responses.” Scientific Reports 11 (1): 20994. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-00188-8.

Cunanan, Aaron J., Brad H. DeWeese, John P. Wagle, Kevin M. Carroll, Robert Sausaman, W. Guy Hornsby, G. Gregory Haff, N. Travis Triplett, Kyle C. Pierce, and Michael H. Stone. 2018. “The General Adaptation Syndrome: A Foundation for the Concept of Periodization.” Sports Medicine 48 (4): 787–97. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-017-0855-3.

“Diathesis–Stress Model.” 2022. In Wikipedia. https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Diathesis%E2%80%93stress_model&oldid=1086665923.

Friston, Karl, Christopher Thornton, and Andy Clark. 2012. “Free-Energy Minimization and the Dark-Room Problem.” Frontiers in Psychology 3. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00130.

Hannibal, Kara E., and Mark D. Bishop. 2014. “Chronic Stress, Cortisol Dysfunction, and Pain: A Psychoneuroendocrine Rationale for Stress Management in Pain Rehabilitation.” Physical Therapy 94 (12): 1816–25. https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20130597.

Kaida, Kosuke, and Kazuhisa Niki. 2013. “Total Sleep Deprivation Decreases Flow Experience and Mood Status.” Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 10 (December): 19–25. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S53633.

Keng, Shian-Ling, Moria J. Smoski, and Clive J. Robins. 2011. “Effects of Mindfulness on Psychological Health: A Review of Empirical Studies.” Clinical Psychology Review 31 (6): 1041–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006.

Liang, Sancai, Meimei Dong, Hongbin Zhao, Yuliang Song, and Anqi Yang. 2022. “Mindfulness and Life Satisfaction: The Moderating Effect of Self-Control and the Moderated Moderating Effect of Resilience.” Personality and Individual Differences 185 (February): 111241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111241.

Mandolesi, Laura, Arianna Polverino, Simone Montuori, Francesca Foti, Giampaolo Ferraioli, Pierpaolo Sorrentino, and Giuseppe Sorrentino. 2018. “Effects of Physical Exercise on Cognitive Functioning and Wellbeing: Biological and Psychological Benefits.” Frontiers in Psychology 9 (April): 509. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00509.

Matthews, Mark, Saeed Abdullah, Geri Gay, and Tanzeem Choudhury. 2014. “Tracking Mental Well-Being: Balancing Rich Sensing and Patient Needs.” Computer 47 (April): 36–43. https://doi.org/10.1109/MC.2014.107.

Meerlo, Peter, Andrea Sgoifo, and Deborah Suchecki. 2008. “Restricted and Disrupted Sleep: Effects on Autonomic Function, Neuroendocrine Stress Systems and Stress Responsivity.” Sleep Medicine Reviews 12 (3): 197–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.007.

Moore, Bryan A. 2013. “Propensity for Experiencing Flow: The Roles of Cognitive Flexibility and Mindfulness.” The Humanistic Psychologist 41 (4): 319–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/08873267.2013.820954.

Mühlen, Jan M., Julie Stang, Esben Lykke Skovgaard, Pedro B. Judice, Pablo Molina-Garcia, William Johnston, Luís B. Sardinha, et al. 2021. “Recommendations for Determining the Validity of Consumer Wearable Heart Rate Devices: Expert Statement and Checklist of the INTERLIVE Network.” British Journal of Sports Medicine 55 (14): 767–79. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2020-103148.

Norris, Richard, Douglas Carroll, and Raymond Cochrane. 1992. “The Effects of Physical Activity and Exercise Training on Psychological Stress and Well-Being in an Adolescent Population.” Journal of Psychosomatic Research 36 (1): 55–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3999(92)90114-H.

Praissman, Sharon. 2008. “Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction: A Literature Review and Clinician’s Guide.” Journal of the American Academy of Nurse Practitioners 20 (4): 212–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-7599.2008.00306.x.

Reardon, Claudia L., Brian Hainline, Cindy Miller Aron, David Baron, Antonia L. Baum, Abhinav Bindra, Richard Budgett, et al. 2019. “Mental Health in Elite Athletes: International Olympic Committee Consensus Statement (2019).” British Journal of Sports Medicine 53 (11): 667–99. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2019-100715.

Robinson, Paula, Lindsay Oades, and Peter Caputi. 2015. “Conceptualising and Measuring Mental Fitness: A Delphi Study.” International Journal of Wellbeing 5 (1). https://www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org/index.php/ijow/article/view/256.

Šimleša, Milija, Jérôme Guegan, Edouard Blanchard, Franck Tarpin-Bernard, and Stéphanie Buisine. 2018. “The Flow Engine Framework: A Cognitive Model of Optimal Human Experience.” Europe’s Journal of Psychology 14 (1): 232–53. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v14i1.1370.

Vitale, Kenneth C., Roberts Owens, Susan R. Hopkins, and Atul Malhotra. 2019. “Sleep Hygiene for Optimizing Recovery in Athletes: Review and Recommendations.” International Journal of Sports Medicine 40 (8): 535–43. https://doi.org/10.1055/a-0905-3103.

Wiese, Christopher W., Lauren Kuykendall, and Louis Tay. 2018. “Get Active? A Meta-Analysis of Leisure-Time Physical Activity and Subjective Well-Being.” The Journal of Positive Psychology 13 (1): 57–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2017.1374436.